Book Chapter: An Integrated Conceptual Framework for Visual Social Research
DATE
19
September 2017
TITLE
An Integrated Conceptual Framework for
Visual Social Research
Book Chapter
REFERENCE
Pauwels, L.
(2011). An integrated conceptual framework for visual social research. In
Margolis, E. & Pauwels, L. The SAGE handbook of visual research
methods (pp. 3-23). London: SAGE Publications Ltd. doi:
10.4135/9781446268278
http://methods.sagepub.com
.ezproxy.aut.ac.nz/book/sage-hdbk-visual-research-methods/n1.xml
MY SUMMARY
Visual outcomes, like mind-mapping, collage,
graffiti, photos, videos, film, drawings, paintings, advertisements, including
in health field. If we notice some research project produces visual outcomes
that are important for process, analysis, and interpretation in order to have
better understanding on some social issues, including in health field. This approach
or method is known as visual social research or visual methods, like Researchers
can generate the visual outcome by themselves, known as ‘researcher-generated
visuals’ or researchers can use other researchers’ visual outcome or visual
outcomes that are available for public, like poster, advertisements,
videos-known as ‘secondary research uses’ and respondents or participants get
involved actively in producing visual outcomes-‘respondent-generated visuals’.
ABSTRACT
CONCLUSIONS
IMPORTANT
QUOTATION IN CHAPTERS
·
Wide use of visual methods in
all research subjects and issues, including in health and gender and identity
At present, many types of societal imagery (for example,
family pictures, ads, postcards, paintings, newsreels, feature and documentary
film, various picture archives, maps, and charts) have been used by social,
cultural, and behavioral scientists to study a variety of subjects and issues:
labor (Margolis, 1994); school culture (Margolis, 2004; Burke and Grosvenor,
2007); family dynamics (Musello, 1979; Chalfen, 1987; Pauwels, 2008a);
traumatic experiences (McAllister, 2006; Gödel, 2007); youth culture (Larson,
1999); stereotyping (Hagaman, 1993); migration (Wright, 2001); nature versus
culture (Papson, 1991; Suonpää, 2000; Bousé, 2003); deviance (Lackey, 2001);
race and ethnicity (Mellinger, 1992; Tomaselli and Shepperson, 2002; Grady,
2007); health (Bogdan and Marshal, 1997); gender and identity (Goffman, 1979;
Edge, 1998); and globalization (Barndt, 1997). However, many areas of enquiry
and many types of visual materials are still waiting to be explored.(p.3)
· Some
terms of how visual outcomes are produced;
o researchers-generated
visuals à researchers have more control for
visual production
o secondary
research uses àvisual
outcome from other researchers) and
o respondent-generated
visuals àrespondents have more control for
visual production with their unique (insider) ‘perspectives’ while researchers
can do further analysis or make sense of their outcomes
Researcher-generated
visuals
Researcher-generated production of visuals in general allows more control
over the data-gathering procedures (and ideally more reflexivity) so that more
highly contextualized material can be produced.
Secondary
Research uses
Researchers may indeed choose to use materials that have
been produced by other researchers for similar or different research purposes.
This material may be used for comparison with new data or (as a historic
source) be revisited by a new researcher for the same purposes or to answer
different research questions
Respondent-generated
visuals
It is important to note that the respondent-generated
material, while offering a unique (insider) perspective, is
never an end product, but just an intermediate step in the research.
Researchers still need to analyze and make sense of the visual output generated
by the respondents; their cultural self-portrayal or vision needs to be
verbally or visually framed within the research output (p.3).
Of course not only naturally occurring or non-reactive behavior
is a valid subject of research; ‘elicited behavior of both a verbal or visual
nature’ may also yield valuable input for research. Researchers can prompt
people to react (most often verbally) to visual stimuli (pictures, drawings,
artifacts) and use these reactions as input or to correct their research
(Collier, 1967; Wagner, 1979; Harper, 2002). Or researchers may even prompt
people to produce their own imagery or visual representations as a response to
a specific assignment (for example, ‘depict a typical day of your life’). The
first technique is known as ‘photo or film elicitation’ (the term ‘visual
elicitation’ may be better, since it does not limit this technique to
photographic media, but also includes drawing, for example) p. 4. The
latter technique whereby the respondents themselves produce imagery or
visual representations about aspects of their culture for further use by the
researcher is (as stated earlier) best described by the broad category of
‘respondent-generated imagery. (p.4)
· A chance
to explain to explain the process of producing visual outcomes to gain better
understanding on actions of visual production
Rituals and other highly
prescribed activities’ in a society offer very condensed information on
important aspects of human organization. Depicting these processes may also
benefit from a visual approach, because of its ability to capture the richness
and complexity of the event, its capacity to cope with the semiotic hybridity
(different types of signs and orders of signification) of the depicted
including its cultural specificity, and development over time and space
(especially when using continuous visual recording techniques: film or video).
·
Diverse visual techniques;
mapping, drawings, paintings, graffiti,
a collage, photo, and film; a map may combine many types of iconic and symbolic information, such as
pictograms, arrows, colors, gradients, and text.
Visual sociologists and anthropologists have primarily
focused on camera-based imagery (both static and moving). The paramount
importance of these kinds of imagery is beyond question, both because of their
ubiquity in society, the ease with which they are produced and because of their
specific iconic and indexical qualities (mostly understood as their high level
of ‘resemblance’ and the ‘natural’ or even
‘causal’ relation to the depicted object). However,
researchers may also take advantage of non-(technically) mediated or directly
observed aspects of visual culture (signage, architecture) and of studying and using non-photographic representations (such as
drawings, paintings, murals, graffiti, maps, charts). In
many cases, ‘fixing the shadows,’ however, by producing a permanent (most often
photographic) record is helpful or even necessary.
Any visual practice and its products embody a complex
meeting of the cultures of the depicted and of the depicter, along with
the—again, culturally influenced—intricacies of the representational techniques
or the medium. Visuals produced with ‘nonalgorithmic techniques’ (techniques
that require many ‘intentional’ choices by the maker, such as drawings:
Mitchell, 1992) are readily used as existing data sources (for example, paintings, murals, graffiti, children's
drawings). For ‘researcher-generated’ types of imagery, however,
this category of imaging techniques is a far less obvious choice. Indeed,
social scientists routinely turn to photography
and film to record material cultural and human
behavior in all of its complexity. Yet in some instances, nonalgorithmic
techniques (more intentional or less automated techniques) can be more suited
or may even prove to be the only option (for example, to depict concepts or
relational constructs as these ‘entities’ cannot be photographed since they
have no visual material referent, or in cases where photography is not
allowed). Intentional techniques, moreover, may be chosen because they allow
simplification and abstraction; photos can be too detailed and particularistic.
Intentional techniques also allow the simultaneous application of many
different representational codes: for example, a map may combine many types of iconic and symbolic information, such as
pictograms, arrows, colors, gradients, and text. The
relation between a picture and its g.h,kghjcvkbn eyjkio` Snaked eye) are used, or when the depicted
cannot be observed directly and thus is only ‘available’ as a representation
(Pauwels, 2006b).
·
Visual method analysis can be undertaken based on the depiction of
visual data and the content of visual data, process of visual data production
and feedbacks of visual data outcomes. In addition, the analysis will depend on
a research questions;
o
The analytical focus will always be
determined by the particular research questions being addressed. These research
questions may cover a vast number of possible areas of research as long as the
right visual angles to answer the questions are found (p.5).
o
In summary, the focus of analysis in visual
research can lie on:
1.
the content of a visual representation (the
depicted),
2.
its form and style (most often in
conjunction with the depicted),
3.
the processes that are related with the
production and use of visual representations,
4.
the verbal reactions to visual stimuli.
o
Researchers always need to be
aware of the inevitable difference between the depicted (the referent) and the
depiction (the visual representation), a difference that can seriously
influence or even misinform their views on the depicted. This difference can
also become a field of study in its own right: the study of style as a gateway
to the norms and values and other immaterial traits of a culture.p.5
o
Operationalizing research questions and
foci from visually observable elements may involve deriving data from images in
a fairly straightforward way (for example, number of people, distances, cultural inventory
of objects) or may require more interpretative decoding (emotional states, complex relations). Such
operationalization may implicate the image or visual field as an integrated
whole (the spatial organization of a town square, the global impression of a
city as a cultural meeting place) or just small parts or aspects of it (clearly
defined types of exchange between people, for example, such as a handshake, eye
contact, or a nod).-p.5
o
B.1.2. Analyzing Production Processes and
Product uses-
1.
Analyzing the processes of image making and
the subsequent uses and cultural practices surrounding the use of imagery and
visual representations are not the most dominant foci in current visual
research, but they too may yield very
unique data. Indeed, in some cases
the process may be more revealing than the end product.
2.
Next to studying the visual end products,
family researchers can also take an interest in the dynamics just before
and during the production of a family snap (the directing, posing, negotiations, the technical choices, and the
implicit power relations) and the processes by which the snaps are afterward
selected, manipulated, and combined with texts in an album or on a website; where, how, and which photos are displayed in
the home or distributed among friends and acquaintances, for what reasons, etc.
(Chalfen, 1987; Pauwels, 2008a).
o
B.1.3. Analysis of Feedback
Visual stimuli are provided by the
researcher to gather factual information about the depicted cultural elements
and—a very powerful and unique trait of the visual elicitation technique—to ‘trigger’
more projective information with the
respondents (their deeper feelings, opinions). The
method of ‘respondent-generated images’ also generates ‘feedback,’ but of a mainly visual nature, and
thus this feedback needs to be analyzed both for its content and its form. It
is to be considered as a research ‘input’ not an end product, even if it takes
the form of a completed film or video. Through detailed analysis, the
researcher will try to make sense of it and situate it within the larger
framework of the discipline.
o
Chosen theory framework will guide
researchers to analyse visual data based on its direction, hypothesis, or ideas
to have better understanding to generate meaningful knowledge.
B.2. Theoretical Foundation
As in most types of research, theory usually guides visual data production and analysis. So
whether looking at existing visual representations or producing new visual
data, both approaches require a solid and fully motivated theoretical
grounding. Without theory, our seeing is blind or tends to rest on unexplained
views and expectations (implicit theory), which we may even be unaware of. It
is fairly naïve to expect that the camera will automatically collect large
quantities of relevant data. Theory is
needed to give scientific research some direction. It can
focus attention on issues which at first sight are not expected to have much
significance, but which from a specific
stance, hypothesis, or idea, can yield relevant scientific information.
However, the theoretical grounding of a project not only
involves the visual analytical side (how to deal with the form and content of
the visual products) but also includes the main subject matter or the thematic
focus of the project. Researchers who, for instance, study gentrification
processes or poverty issues start by selecting particular definitions and
aspects of gentrification or poverty theories and research, and combine those
in a solid framework that is compatible with the goals of the research and with
the particular combination of research methods and techniques.
B.3.4. Nature and Degree of Field
Involvement
People may, however, react to being
recorded whether or not they know its exact purpose: they may try to hide away,
or to perform in front of the camera in less or more explicit ways. When people
know they are being recorded they most often display a degree of reactivity.
Looking into the camera is the most noticeable, but not necessarily the most
significant reaction. This reactivity may even be, or become, the very subject
matter of the research.
Many visual researchers have
experienced the value of involving the field in a more active and encompassing
way (not just during the recording, but before and afterward), which can lead to more
‘participatory and joint forms of production’ (Rouch, 1975). In
fact, sometimes this participation of the community under study may be the main
objective of the project, which then, rather than having a scientific purpose,
seeks to promote community empowerment or activism. In this
case, the researcher helps the community realize its goals rather than vice
versa, which is normally the case.
Reflexcivity
These basic requirements today form
part and parcel of a broader call for reflexivity in science, which
entails a clear recognition that all knowledge is ‘work in progress,’
incomplete, and perspectivistic (see also Rosaldo, 1989; Ruby, 2000; Pauwels,
2006c). With respect to visual research, reflexivity in particular involves
giving a concrete shape to the idea that research is a complex ‘meeting of
cultures’ (MacDougall, 1975: 119): to start with the cultures of the
researchers (personal beliefs, preferences, experiences, characteristics,
cultural backgrounds) and those of the researched, and at a later stage with
the cultural stance of the viewers or users of the resulting visual product
Comments
Post a Comment